Categorization and Concept
Development
n
How do you know these are all cats?
Or all of these are animals
n
Classic view of categories was that they could be
defined by simple criterial features
n
For example, participants would learn to categorize :
Research by Rosch
in 70’s
n
Members of categories are not equally representative
but have typicality structures
n
Reaction Times to identify typical items are faster
than atypical items
n
Children learn the names for typical items faster than
atypical items
Research by Rosch
in 70’s
n
Members of categories are not equally
representative but have typicality structures
What about individuals with
autism
Category Membership is Determined by typicality
n
Prototype Models (e.g, Homa)
n
Exemplar Models (e.g., Medin,
Nosofsky)
How do we learn categories?
n
Over last 20 years there have been numerous studies
with infants (e.g., Cohen, Strauss, Quinn, Younger) using habituation paradigm.
How would you come to learn
this “alien” world
n
Fortunately, infants appear to have inborn mechanisms
that help then learn and figure out the world.
n
They innately abstract generalized representations of
categories
n
Imagine you’ve never seen a llama before. Someone shows you 1 then 2, then 3….examples
n
Eventually, you’ll have abstracted a general idea of
what llamas look like
What about individuals with
autism?
n
While typically developing individuals respond faster
to typical than atypical examples, this is not true of individuals with autism.
n
When asked to rate the typicality of items, their
conceptions are quite different than controls
n
They do not seem to have abstracted what “average” or prototypical
objects look like. Their notions of
categories depend on idiosyncratic details.
What about more abstract
concepts?
n
Friendship, love, play?
n
We speculate that individuals with autism also do not
abstract generalized representations of these concepts. Rather, they define them in very detailed,
perhaps idiosyncratic ways.
What is the role of language
n
Research suggests categorization comes prior to
language
n
Then why do infants do over-extensions?
n
Does language play any role
n
Maybe for some categories?
n
Way of “motivating” infant to make distinction
Role of perceptual vs.
conceptual information
n
Researcher have argued about which is primary
n
Perhaps both simultaneously?
What about quantity concepts?
n
Can infants discriminate discrete quantities?
Discrimination of Quantity
Heterogeneous Condition
2 dogs
2 chairs
2 books
2 cars
.
.
3 tables
2 pencils
Results
n
Babies starting at 5 months of age can discriminate
quantities of 2 vs 3, 3 vs
4 but not 4 vs 5.
n
Why
Subitizing
Subitizing
n
Cross cultural research: Amzonian
Indians
n
What’s it’s function
n
In animals
n
Underlying later skills
Ordinal Abilities
n
The ability to understand more versus less
n
With both continuous and discrete information
Results
n
16 month olds are able to learn task and always pick
the larger (or smaller) quantity. They
don’t seem to pay attention to the exact quantities
n
12 month olds are unable to do the task
n
Why?
Results
n
With continuous quantities, 12 month olds can do the
task
n
What’s going on?
n
Perhaps they’re learning discrete amounts from their
experiences with large or long things.
n
Consider Piaget’s number conservation task
Can infants add and subtract?
n
Karen Wynn’s study:
Knowing the physical world
n
Piaget and other constructivist
assumed our knowledge comes from active interactions and explorations of the
world
n
This is especially true in infants
older than 7 months as crawling develops and infants become more physical
capable
n
Others (e.g., E. Spelke
and R. Baillargeon) have argued that some knowledge
appears very early (4 to 5 months) suggesting an innate basis similar to
Gibson’s views of perception.
n
An alternative position is that
babies are observing from an
early age and developing expectations
Leslie’s Causality
n
Contact-Immediate to Non Contact- Delay
Causal to Non Causal (2 elements)
Contact- Delay to
Non Contact- Immediate
Non
Causal to Non Causal (2
elements)
Causality: Studies by A. Leslie
n
Habituate to:
Contact and Immediate (Casual)
Test
n
No Contact and Delay (Non- Casual)
Causality: Studies by A. Leslie
n
Habituate to:
Contact and Delay ( Non-Casual)
Test
n
No Contact and Immediate (Non- Casual)
Leslie
n
Results:
Infants perceived causality at 7 months
n
While he originally suggested an innate module
n
Is this necessary?
n
Follow-up research by L. Cohen
n
Knowledge is somewhat fragile—small parametric changes
will lead to different results
n
Not until closer to 10 months is it robost
Object Permanence
n
R. Bailargeon
Object Permanence
Object Permanence
n
L. Cohen has argued that infants are really just more
interested in the full screen condition because they have not fully habituated
n
Other’s disagree and believe that there is some innate
knowledge of permanence
n
An alternative may be that infants have some visual
expectations but conceptually do not understand permanence
E. Spelke: Object
Solidity
Object Solidity
Again…..
n
L. Cohen has argued and demonstrated that there are
other “perceptual” explanations for the results and that this knowledge does
not exist until 10 months of age
n
Conclusions?
What is significance of infancy?
n
Period of time where we learn about the “sensori-motor” world